LeBron James exploded past his defender and raced towards the lane.
Serge Ibaka, the Thunder’s mountainous center, planted his feet and raised his hands straight up into the air. LeBron ducked his left shoulder and plowed right into Ibaka, who went crashing backwards into a nearby cameraman.
Offensive foul?
Maybe if it had been the first quarter. But given that this was the last minutes of a tightly fought game, the referees chose to restrain themselves, not wanting the game to turn on their actions. Was this even controversial? Not a bit. In such situations, announcers typically applaud the non-call, intoning platitudes like “this game should be decided by the players.”
In their excellent book Scorecasting, Tobias Moskowitz and L. Jon Woertheim explore the psychology of sports through exhaustive and yet entertaining analyses of all kinds of topics that have fueled many a heated bar stool argument.
Are referees biased against your favorite team? According to their analyses, they are biased against your team only if it is playing an away game. Turns out that their unconscious desires to please fans cause referees and umpires to back away from controversial calls that will raise the crowd’s ire.
One of the most fascinating chapters in the book involves what the authors call “whistle swallowing.” All else equal, referees and umpires avoid sins of commission over sins of omission, a preference for inactivity nicely summarized by veteran NBA referee Gary Benson: “It’s late in the game and, let’s say, there’s goal tending and you miss it. That’s an incorrect non-call and that’s bad. But let’s say it’s late in the game and you call goal tending on a play and the re-play shows it was an incorrect call. That’s when you’re in a really deep mess.”
Those of us who study medical decision making are well aware of this preference for inactivity. We see it every time a patient chooses to forego a flu shot out of fear that they will experience side effects from the vaccine, even though the chance of side effects pales in comparison to the vaccine’s benefits. In one study, a substantial number of people said they would rather accept a 10% chance of dying from the flu than receive a vaccine that carried a 5% risk of causing a fatal side effect.
This powerful phenomenon, this omission bias, causes baseball umpires to enlarge the strike zone by 93 square inches on 3-0 counts, so they won’t influence the game by walking the batter. It also cause NBA referees to disproportionately call loose ball fowls on non-stars, so the decisions won’t take stars like Kobe and Carmelo out of the game and thereby influence the outcome.
Those of us who work in medical decision making know this preference for inaction well. But we also know some major exceptions to this behavior. Chief among them: the way physicians think about action and inaction when contemplating ways they can avoid being sued for malpractice.
I have encountered this thinking scores of times when speaking to medical audiences about the importance of controlling healthcare spending. In my talks, I tell stories of testing run amok, and of procedures ordered in an almost willy nilly fashion. Inevitably an audience member, a physician, will rush to the microphone at the end of my talk and vehemently explain: “That’s fine to control costs and all. But meanwhile I’m going to get sued because my patient develops a tumor that I didn’t find because I didn’t order a CT scan six months ago when they had milder symptoms.”
According to health economists, practicing defensive medicine adds billions of dollars to our healthcare budget, with physicians ordering low yield tests so the patients will realize their doctors have done everything possible to explain every one of their symptoms.
I understand the psychological power of lawsuits. Indeed, some companies make a decent living helping physicians overcome the emotional trauma of being sued. Even unsuccessful litigation can take an emotional toll, causing doctors to question their own judgment and to lose trust in their patients.
But why does fear of litigation necessarily turn into preference for action? After all, that unnecessary CT scan could reveal a suspicious “density,” which leads to an unnecessary biopsy, the latter test potentially causing an unexpected complication. Isn’t it possible that your patient will sue you for chasing after imaginary shadows? What evidence is there that aggressive physicians—those who order lots of tests and procedures—are sued any less often than their more parsimonious colleagues?
What we have here—both for NBA referees and experienced physicians—is the triumph of psychology over professional expertise. Preference for both action and inaction needs to be squelched, in favor of preference for the truth. Professionals need to become more aware of their own biases, so they can better sort out offensive fouls and defensive medicine.
Peter Ubel is a physician, behavioral scientist and author of Pricing Life: Why It’s Time for Health Care Rationing and Free Market Madness. He teaches business and public policy at Duke University. Peter’s new book, Critical Decisions will be available in the fall of 2012. You can follow him on his personal blog.
Categories: Uncategorized
Excellent article- I enjoyed the original perspective. I want to add one thing: Defensive medicine is also an error of omission. It represents an omission of informed consent. No informed patient would agree to a pointless dose of radiation, and physicians that have obtained and documented a patient’s informed decision not to utilize such unnecessary tests can not be held liable for malpractice.
It’s quite obvious that juries will favor action i/o inaction – ordering the wrong
Tests for a challemging dx is better tjan ordering none at all.
Um, not buying this line of reasoning. I have never heard of a case of being sued for “chasing after imaginary shadows.” I’m sure such litigation has happened, but not so much as the other – being sued for not doing the test – you can read about those cases pretty much every week.
There is not a physician out there who has not said, “Let’s give this another week or two and if your not better, we’ll order a _______.” Then, two weeks later, if you don’t order _______ you’re screwed. But, if you hadn’t promised to do something for failure to improve, you also would have been potentially liable. So yeah, we put off a few unnecessary tests, but until you are protected for following standard of care there will be no incentive not to exceed it.
“loose ball fowls”
Hmmm…
http://img.thesun.co.uk/multimedia/archive/01504/chick_22_532x360_1504358a.jpg
Given our technology, I really don’t see why we have home plate umps anymore. Or, minimally, as they do in other sports, a limited number of “challenges” for instant replay.
__
“What we have here—both for NBA referees and experienced physicians—is the triumph of psychology over professional expertise. Preference for both action and inaction needs to be squelched, in favor of preference for the truth. Professionals need to become more aware of their own biases, so they can better sort out offensive fouls and defensive medicine.”
Indeed. I could not be more sympathetic the the clinicians’ dilemma.